#number

kuchinster@rusx.org

About Russian Air Defense

Moreover, those gargantuan arrays of combat information are the foundation for Russian air defense systems global domination for decades to come. This combat and scientific-engineering experience is priceless, you cannot buy it. They threw at Russia and her people everything combined West had, with the exception of the Intercontinental Ballistic Missiles, they failed because more than 95% of it was intercepted. This is unprecedented.

#russian #weapons is #Westerrn #NATO #US #Pentagon Great #fail on #failstate

Image/photoSol wrote the following post Tue, 13 Aug 2024 02:01:39 +0300

The Number Is Shocking...

The #Number Is #Shocking ... #Russia #Air #Defense and #Anti-Missile #systems #Ukraine

nowisthetime@pod.automat.click

ex-British Policeman @XPCBirmingham Mark Sexton 8th March 2024:

#CRIME #NUMBER 01/62447/24 issued at Acton poo lice station London at 23.13 hours Friday 8th of March #2024 for Misconduct in Public Office, Misfeasance in Public Office, Gross Negligent Manslaughter, Corporate Manslaughter and Fraud by False Representation.

Allegations made against Sir Graham Brady MP, Dame June Raine, The MHRA, Pfizer and the U.K Government.

anonymiss@despora.de

The #Number #Five Is Nothing Like the Number #Four

Our #brains #process them in completely different ways.

source: https://www.theatlantic.com/science/archive/2023/12/processing-numbers-four-five-biology/676380/?gift=bt1n2vDjnfxxzkH_t7B8m4IBC69GwvvR7NuTR_ScRwg

More than 150 years ago, the economist and philosopher William Stanley Jevons discovered something curious about the number four. While musing about how the mind conceives of numbers, he tossed a handful of black beans into a cardboard box. Then, after a fleeting glance, he guessed how many there were, before counting them to record the true value. After more than 1,000 trials, he saw a clear pattern. When there were four or fewer beans in the box, he always guessed the right number. But for five beans or more, his quick estimates were often incorrect.

Jevons’s description of his self-experiment, published in Nature in 1871, set the “foundation of how we think about numbers,” Steven Piantadosi, a professor of psychology and neuroscience at UC Berkeley, says. It sparked a long-lasting and ongoing debate about why there seems to be a limit on the number of items we can accurately judge to be present in a set.

Now a new study in Nature Human Behaviour has edged closer to an answer by taking an unprecedented look at how human brain cells fire when presented with certain quantities. Its findings suggest that the brain uses a combination of two mechanisms to judge how many objects it sees. One estimates quantities. The second sharpens the accuracy of those estimates—but only for small numbers.

It’s “very exciting” that the findings connect long-contested ideas to their neural underpinnings, Piantadosi, who was not involved in the study, says. “There’s not many things in cognition where people have been able to pinpoint very plausible biological foundations.” Although the new study does not end the debate, the findings start to untangle the biological basis for how the brain judges quantities, which could inform bigger questions about memory, attention, and even mathematics.

The ability to instantly judge the number of items in a set doesn’t have anything to do with counting. Human infants have this number sense even before they learn language. And it’s not limited to humans: Monkeys, bees, fish, crows, and other animals also have it. A monkey needs to be able to quickly judge the number of apples in a tree, and also how many other monkeys it’s competing against for those apples. A lion, when confronted by other lions, has to decide whether to fight or flee. Honeybees need to know which area has the most flowers for foraging. A guppy has better chances of escaping a predator if it joins a shoal. “The bigger the shoal, the safer that little fish is,” Brian Butterworth, a cognitive neuroscientist at University College London who was not involved in the new work, says.

This innate number sense is therefore crucial to survival, increasing an animal’s chances of finding food, avoiding predators, and ultimately reproducing. “It simply pays off for the survival of an animal to be able to differentiate numeric quantities,” says Andreas Nieder, a professor of animal physiology at the University of Tübingen, in Germany, who co-led the new study. The fact that this ability is found in diverse animals, from insects to humans, suggests that it arose a long time ago, and its neural basis has interested cognitive scientists for decades.

In 2002, when Nieder was working with the neuroscientist Earl Miller at MIT as a postdoctoral fellow, they published one of the first pieces of evidence that numbers are linked to specific neurons. In a behavioral experiment using monkeys, they found that these neurons, which are located in the prefrontal cortex, where higher-level processing takes place, have preferred numbers—favorite numbers that, when perceived, make the cells light up in brain scans.

For example, some neurons are tuned to the number three. When they’re presented with three objects, they fire more. Other neurons are tuned to the number five and fire when presented with five objects, and so on. These neurons aren’t exclusively committed to their favorites: They also fire for numbers adjacent to them. (So the neuron tuned to five also fires for four and six objects.) But they don’t do it as often, and as the presented number gets further away from the preferred number, the neurons’ firing rate decreases.

Nieder was excited by the deeper questions the work presented about the development of mathematical ability. Numbers lead to counting, and then to symbolic number representations, such as Arabic numerals that stand in for quantities. Those symbolic numbers underpin arithmetic and mathematics. “For us to know how numbers are represented [in the brain] is setting the foundation for everything that’s coming later,” Nieder says.

He went on to learn as much as he could about number neurons. In 2012, his team discovered that the neurons respond to their preferred numbers when they’re estimating the quantity of a set of sounds or visual items. Then, in 2015, they showed that crows also have number neurons. In a show of “amazing crow behavior,” Nieder says, the birds could correctly peck the number of dots or Arabic numerals displayed to them.

However, no one had identified number neurons in humans. That’s because studying the human brain is notoriously difficult: Scientists usually can’t access its activity ethically in experiments while people are alive. Brain-imaging tools don’t have the resolution needed to distinguish individual neurons, and scientific curiosity alone can’t justify implanting invasive electrodes in the brain.

To peer into a living brain, Nieder needed to find patients who already had electrode implants and who would consent to being part of his research. In 2015, he contacted Florian Mormann—the head of the cognitive and clinical neurophysiology group at the University of Bonn, who is one of the few clinicians in Germany who does single-cell recordings in human patients—to see if he and his patients would join Nieder’s search for human number neurons. Mormann said yes, and their teams got to work examining the brain activity of his epilepsy patients, who had previously had electrodes implanted to improve their medical care.

Nine patients did simple calculations in their heads while researchers recorded their brain activity. Sure enough, in the data, Nieder and Mormann saw neurons firing for their preferred numbers—the first time number neurons had been identified in the human brain. They published their findings in Neuron in 2018.

Neuroscientists are of course driven to understand the mind, Nieder says, so “finding such neurons in the human brain is extremely rewarding.” To continue their quest, Nieder and Mormann launched a new study to find out how the neurons represent odd and even numbers. The researchers recruited 17 epilepsy patients and showed them flashes of dots, ranging in number from one to nine, on computer screens. The participants indicated whether they saw an odd or even number while electrodes recorded their brain activity.

Over the next few months, as Esther Kutter, a graduate student studying with Nieder, analyzed the resulting data, she saw a clear pattern emerge—right around the number four. The data, which comprised 801 recordings of single neurons firing, showed two distinct neural signatures: one for small numbers and one for large. Above the number four, the neurons’ firing for their preferred number grew progressively less precise, and they erroneously fired for numbers close to the preferred one. But for four and below, the neurons fired precisely—with the same small amount of error whether firing for one, two, three, or four objects. The misfiring in response to other numbers was largely absent.

This surprised Nieder. He hadn’t previously seen this boundary in his animal studies: Those experiments had included numbers only up to five. He hadn’t set out to probe Jevons’s observation, nor did he expect to see a neural boundary confirm what behavioral studies had found. Up until that point, he had been convinced that the brain had just one mechanism for judging numbers—a continuum that got fuzzier the higher the numbers climbed.

The new data changed that for him. “This boundary popped out in different ways,” Nieder says. The neural patterns suggested that there is an additional mechanism that suppresses smaller-number neurons from firing for the wrong numbers. Piantadosi and Serge Dumoulin, the director of the Spinoza Centre for Neuroimaging, in Amsterdam, had both previously published papers supporting the idea that only one mechanism manages the neuronal interpretation of numbers. Yet they were struck by Nieder and Mormann’s new data showing that there are, in fact, two separate mechanisms.

It’s “real validation that large and small numbers have different neural signatures,” Piantadosi says. But he cautioned that two signatures can emerge from a single process; whether it should be described as one mechanism or two is still up for debate. “This is just beautiful,” Dumoulin says. “This type of data wasn’t available, and certainly not in humans.”

However, one more major uncertainty remains. The researchers didn’t study the prefrontal or parietal cortices, where the majority of number neurons are located in monkeys. Instead, because of where the patients’ electrodes were inserted, the study focused on the medial temporal lobe, which is involved in memory. It isn’t the first place in the human brain you’d investigate to understand numbers, Nieder says. “On the other hand, the medial temporal lobe is also not the worst place to look for such neurons.”

That’s because the medial temporal lobe is linked to number sense. It’s active when children learn calculations and multiplication tables, and it’s intimately connected to regions where number neurons are thought to lie, Nieder says. It’s not clear why number neurons are present in this region, Butterworth says. “The things that we thought were specific to the parietal lobe seem to be reflected also in parts of the medial temporal lobe.”

One possibility is that these aren’t number neurons at all. Pedro Pinheiro-Chagas, an assistant professor of neurology at UC San Francisco, thinks these could instead be concept neurons, which are located in the medial temporal lobe and are each linked to specific concepts. For example, one famous study found a concept neuron that responded directly and specifically to images of the actor Jennifer Aniston. “Maybe they are not finding the mechanisms of the number sense … Maybe they’re finding concept cells that are also applied to numbers,” Pinheiro-Chagas says. “As you have the concept of Jennifer Aniston, you could have the concept of three.”

The level of analysis is “just really outstanding,” says Marinella Cappelletti, a cognitive neuroscientist at Goldsmiths, University of London. The researchers provide “compelling evidence” for dual mechanisms in the medial temporal lobe. She thinks it would be valuable, however, to see whether these mechanisms operate in other brain regions as well, if the opportunity presents itself. “I see these findings as looking into a window,” Cappelletti says. “It would be nice to open it up a bit more and tell us more about the rest of the brain.”

The new findings have clear parallels to the limitations of working memory. People can hold only a certain number of objects in their awareness, or working memory, at one time. Experiments show that number is also four. The agreement between the boundary of number sense and that of working memory is “hard to ignore,” Cappelletti says.

The mechanisms may be related. In previous studies of number sense, when a participant stopped paying attention, they lost their ability to precisely judge the true value of numbers four and below. That suggests that the small-number system, which suppresses adjacent misfirings with small numbers, might be intimately tied to attention. Nieder now hypothesizes that the small-number system turns on only when you’re paying attention to what’s in front of you. He’s hoping to test this idea in monkeys, in addition to looking for a neural boundary at four that their experiments haven’t yet captured.

The latest research “seems to be the beginning of a new leap” in our understanding of number perception, Pinheiro-Chagas says, which could have useful applications. He hopes it will be fodder for discussions in math education and even artificial intelligence, which struggles with numerosity perception. Large language models are “pretty bad at counting,” he says. “They are pretty bad at understanding quantities.”

Better characterizing number neurons can also help us understand who we are. Next to the language system, number representation is humans’ second-biggest symbol system. People use numbers frequently and in a variety of ways, and we and our ancestors have used math to describe the world for millennia. In that sense, math is a fundamental part of being human. And, as this study starts to show, this calculation prowess might all stem from a finely tuned network of neurons in the #brain.

#science #mathematics #education #news #numbers

quetzop1@diasp.org

Internet

I'm really annoyed by the #Internet of today:

  • #Trackers and #data #collection everywhere
  • #JavaScript-heavy #Web #applications instead of document-oriented #websites
  • No #JavaScript most often translates to an empty page with a single sentence: "Please activate JavaScript"; the page content however is often nothing that actually requires JavaScript, the website creators just want to feel like actual #application #developers, so they re-build much of what the #browser already supplies with #inefficient and #bug heavy JavaScript code
  • Content almost always behind a #login wall
  • More often than not only very superficial #information
  • #Ads
  • Thousands of 3rd party JS files included, most of which have the only purpose of tracking you across websites
  • #Misinformation and #biased #information everywhere
  • Deliberately misleading advertisment, sich as "save 80% now", and artificial time pressure)
  • "Best viewed on #Google #Chrome"
  • "Login with Facebook"
  • Newsletter subscription and cookie pop-ups featuring #dark #patterns
  • #Search #engine #optimization ( #SEO ) acts in the worst interest of the user by skewing search results
  • Artificial restriction of web #app functionality to promote their native apps
  • Large parts of the Web are only accessible by #smartphone
  • You have to provide your #phone #number to login
  • If you didn't provide a phone number, your account is being blocked right after the initial login because we suspect you being malicious actor because why not (=> #Instagram, #Facebook)
  • #Proprietary #platforms are required to participate in public #online life (Amazon, Google, Facebook, Instagram, Twitter, YouTube)
  • One-sentence-paragraphs and sloppy language (especially found in #Medium #articles)
  • "We care about your #privacy" actually means: "We were forced by law to do this shit, we just want to collect and store as much information on you as possible to make money off of you now or in an undescript future"
  • JavaScript code minimizer
  • Large font sizes, much whitespace, large illustrative, but useless images, HD screen required to browse most websites
  • Lack of #government #regulation and #law #enforcement, too many malicious actors (#spam, #phishing, etc.)
  • Emotional content to increase #interaction, #clickbait

Once being an open platform geared towards information exchange and bringing people into contact, most of the public Internet today is nothing but annoying useless #marketing, #advertising and #data #collection. Providing information, connecting people, and making life convenient is definitely NOT the primary goal of whoever is big on the Internet today. It's shocking to see how much of it is only to sell you stuff or to sell your information.

And the worst is: we are even paying them to do this shit. #Marketing spending will be reflected in product prices, and with much of marketing being done in 1st world countries, a substantial amount of the price goes into this destructive industry.

I could go on with this for hours. Really sick of it.

ramnath@nerdpol.ch

Are YOU #Being #Activated? Here Are the Signs to Look For | #Mary #Rodwell

#MaryRodwell shares her observations about what is happening to millions of people around the world during what she and others have called the great awakening. Many are reporting not only physical symptoms like ear buzzing, dizziness and palpitations, but also regular brushes with the #paranormal, #synchronicity and witnessing LOTS of repeating #number #patterns (i.e. 11:11).

Mary not only feels that indeed we are seeing the signs of a planetary and species wide #frequency #acceleration but also a possible plan to stop this evolutionary leap in its tracks.

Published 27th July 2022 (10:06)

Source: https://youtube.com/watch?v=woaXWSwqNg4

Are YOU Being Activated? Here Are the Signs to Look For | Mary Rodwell

hudsonlacerda@diasporabr.com.br

Science Integrity Digest: Concerns about Marseille’s IHUMI/AMU papers – Part 2

This is part 2 of a series describing papers from the #IHUMI / #AMU institutions in #Marseille, #France, with potential problems. In Part 1, I listed papers with #image concerns. In this follow-up I’ll focus on a set of #papers that might have problems with #human subjects research not having received proper #ethical #approval. The articles span a #decade of research on #homeless people in Marseille, and involve #different studies and specimens — but all were run #under #just #one #IRB #approval #number.

https://scienceintegritydigest.com/2021/08/31/concerns-about-marseilles-ihumi-amu-papers-part-2/#more-2672

#science #ethics #medicine #research